The article puts forth a thesis that usurping the right to create objectively existing
symbolic order relies on brining the past and the future into a closed social system
with an imperatively imposed effectively measurable set of empirical attributes
occurring in the experiment, corresponds to the tendency of not caring about
human experiences and limitations imposed by the conscience and, conversely,
taking into account only the capacity for behaviors caused by natural intellectual
abilities of man. Formally speaking S. Milgram’s experiments into blind obedience
to authority, P. Zimbardo‘s Stanford Prison Experiment as well as R. Jones’
experiment into the simulated Nazi experience have shown the destructive
results of being subjected to an arbitrary system of commands in which neither
the expression of an experience nor the principal duty driven by the good of
others have any meaning. Within an authoritarian system subjugation of people
is foremost regardless of who they are; in a totalitarian system submission to
authority is complete, the symbolic man becomes unnecessary, the experience
occurring within social history is negated and, in consequence, it ceases to be
the basis of material ethics. The usurpation of the right to objectivity outside of
the symbolic tradition occurs in antisocial personality. The egotistical style of
behavior in sociopathy consists of the selfish adaptation to the environment to take
advantage of others – to laze, parasitize and waste. The spreading of the separatist
tendency in development and upbringing influences the adaptation of traits of
egotism and selfishness by people who have different personality structures.
REFERENCES(33)
1.
Arendt, H. (2004a). Eichmann w Jerozolimie. Rzecz o banalności zła, Kraków: Znak.
Asch, J.E. (2001). Wpływ nacisku grupy na zmianę i zniekształcenie postaw. W: J. Siuta (red.), Konteksty ludzkich zachowań, Kraków: Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Jagiellońskiego, s. 17–30.
Bernier, A., Carlson, S.M. i Whipple, N. (2010). From external regulation to selfregulation: Early parenting precursors of young children’s executive functioning, „Child Development” nr 81(1), s. 326–339.
Kahneman, D. i Snell, J. (1992). Predicting a changing taste: Do people know what they will like?, „Journal of Behavioral Decision Making” nr 5(3), s. 187–200.
Raszkiewicz, H. (2008). Semiotyczna próżnia – myślenie po totalitaryzmie. W: A. Jabłoński, M. Zemło (red.), Między unifikacją a dezintegracją, Lublin: Wydawnictwo KUL, s. 175–227.
Raszkiewicz, H. (2015). Symboliczne wyrażenie i znakowa mediacja ustosunkowania do obiektu. Psychologia przeżycia i działania z perspektywy symbolu i znaku, Warszawa: Wydawnictwo SGGW.
Staub, E. (2014). Obeying, joining, following, resisting, and others processes in the Milgram studies, and in the holocaust and other genocides: Situations, personality and bystanders, „Journal of Social Issues”, nr 70, s. 501–514.
We process personal data collected when visiting the website. The function of obtaining information about users and their behavior is carried out by voluntarily entered information in forms and saving cookies in end devices. Data, including cookies, are used to provide services, improve the user experience and to analyze the traffic in accordance with the Privacy policy. Data are also collected and processed by Google Analytics tool (more).
You can change cookies settings in your browser. Restricted use of cookies in the browser configuration may affect some functionalities of the website.